Blog Archive

Monday 26 May 2008

http://www.ajph.org/cgi/content/full/93/8/1261

AJPH
HOME HELP FEEDBACK SUBSCRIPTIONS ARCHIVE SEARCH TABLE OF CONTENTS
 QUICK SEARCH: [advanced]




This Article
Right arrow Abstract Freely available
Right arrow Full Text (PDF)
Right arrow Submit a response
Right arrow View responses
Right arrow Purchase Article
Right arrow View Shopping Cart
Right arrow Alert me when this article is cited
Right arrow Alert me when eLetters are posted
Right arrow Alert me if a correction is posted
Services
Right arrow Similar articles in this journal
Right arrow Similar articles in ISI Web of Science
Right arrow Similar articles in PubMed
Right arrow Alert me to new issues of the journal
Right arrow Download to citation manager
Right arrow reprints & permissions
Citing Articles
Right arrow Citing Articles via HighWire
Right arrow Citing Articles via ISI Web of Science (153)
Right arrow Citing Articles via Google Scholar
Google Scholar
Right arrow Articles by Glasgow, R. E.
Right arrow Articles by Marcus, A. C.
Right arrow Search for Related Content
PubMed
Right arrow PubMed Citation
Right arrow Articles by Glasgow, R. E.
Right arrow Articles by Marcus, A. C.
Related Collections
Right arrow Health Promotion
Right arrow Prevention
Right arrow Health Education
August 2003, Vol 93, No. 8 | American Journal of Public Health 1261-1267
© 2003 American Public Health Association


PUBLIC HEALTH MATTERS

Why Don’t We See More Translation of Health Promotion Research to Practice? Rethinking the Efficacy-to-Effectiveness Transition

Russell E. Glasgow, PhD, Edward Lichtenstein, PhD and Alfred C. Marcus, PhD

Russell E. Glasgow and Alfred C. Marcus are with Kaiser Permanente Colorado and AMC Cancer Research Center, Denver. Edward Lichtenstein is with the Oregon Research Institute, Eugene.

Correspondence: Requests for reprints should be sent to Russell E. Glasgow, PhD, PO Box 349, Canon City, CO 81215 (e-mail: russg@ris.net).


 ABSTRACT
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

The gap between research and practice is well documented. We address one of the underlying reasons for this gap: the assumption that effectiveness research naturally and logically follows from successful efficacy research. These 2 research traditions have evolved different methods and values; consequently, there are inherent differences between the characteristics of a successful efficacy intervention versus those of an effectiveness one. Moderating factors that limit robustness across settings, populations, and intervention staff need to be addressed in efficacy studies, as well as in effectiveness trials. Greater attention needs to be paid to documenting intervention reach, adoption, implementation, and maintenance. Recommendations are offered to help close the gap between efficacy and effectiveness research and to guide evaluation and possible adoption of new programs.


 INTRODUCTION
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

Despite a growing literature documenting prevention and health promotion interventions that have proven successful in well-controlled research, few of these interventions are consistently implemented in applied settings. This is true across preventive counseling services for numerous target behaviors, including tobacco use, dietary change, physical activity, and behavioral health issues (e.g., alcohol use, depression). Several recent reviews and metaanalyses have documented this gap,1,2 and the task forces on both clinical preventive services and community preventive services have noted that in several areas there is insufficient applied evidence available to make recommendations at present.3–5 Most of the Healthy People 2000 objectives6 were not met, and the even more ambitious goals in Healthy People 2010 are similarly unlikely to be met without significant changes in the status quo.7,8 To meet these challenges, we will need to have substantially more demonstrations of how to effectively implement recommendations in typical settings and in locations serving minority, low-income, and rural populations facing health disparities.

This situation is not unique to preventive interventions, as strikingly documented in the recent Institute of Medicine report Crossing the Chasm,9 which summarizes the similar state of affairs regarding many medical and disease management interventions. For example, there is increasing consensus on evidence-based diabetes management practices to prevent complications and on the importance and costeffectiveness of these practices.10 However, these recommendations—and especially those related to lifestyle counseling and behavioral issues—are poorly implemented in practice.11–14

This gap between research and practice is the result of several interacting factors, including limited time and resources of practitioners, insufficient training,15 lack of feedback and incentives for use of evidence-based practices, and inadequate infrastructure and systems organization to support translation.8,16 In this article, we focus on another reason for the slow and incomplete translation of research findings into practice: the logic and assumptions behind the design of efficacy and effectiveness research trials.


 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS TRIALS
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

Many of the methods used in current prevention science are based on 2 influential papers published in the 1980s: Greenwald and Cullen’s17 description of the phases of cancer control research and Flay’s analysis of efficacy and effectiveness research.18 Both papers argued for a logical progression of research designs through which promising intervention ideas should proceed. These papers had many positive effects in helping to establish prevention research and enhancing acceptability among other disciplines. However, they may also have had an important and inadvertent negative consequence that derives from the assumption that the best candidates for effectiveness studies—and later dissemination—are interventions that prove successful in certain types of efficacy research. We argue that this assumption, or at least the way in which it has been operationalized over the past 15 years, has often led to interventions that have low probability of success in real-world settings.

To understand this point, it is necessary first to briefly review the seminal papers by Flay18 and Greenwald and Cullen.17 Efficacy trials are defined by Flay as a test of whether a "program does more good than harm when delivered under optimum conditions."18(p451) Efficacy trials are characterized by strong control in that a standardized program is delivered in a uniform fashion to a specific, often narrowly defined, homogeneous target audience. Owing to the strict standardization of efficacy trials, any positive (or negative) effect can be directly attributed to the intervention being studied.

Effectiveness trials are defined as a test of whether a "program does more good than harm when delivered under real-world conditions."18(p451) They typically standardize availability and access among a defined population while allowing implementation and levels of participation to vary on the basis of real-world conditions. The primary goal of an effectiveness trial is to determine whether an intervention works among a broadly defined population. Effectiveness trials that result in no change may be the result of a lack of proper implementation or weak acceptance or adherence by participants.18,19

Greenwald and Cullen17 proposed 5 phases of intervention research presumed to unfold in a sequential fashion. This continuum begins with Phase I research to formulate and develop intervention hypotheses for future study. Phase II studies develop methodologies that can be used in future efficacy or effectiveness studies. Phase III (efficacy) studies test intervention hypotheses, using methods that have been tested in Phase II. Thus, Phase III studies are designed to test interventions for efficacy, with an emphasis on internal validity, the purpose of which is to establish a causal link between the intervention and outcomes. Given this emphasis on internal control, Greenwald and Cullen note that Phase III studies can be conducted in settings and with samples that will "optimize interpretation of efficacy," including study samples that may be more homogeneous than the ultimate target population, and settings that will maximize management of and control over the research process.

The main objective of Phase IV (effectiveness) studies is to measure the impact of an intervention when it is tested within a population that is representative of the intended target audience. Given that Phase IV studies should yield results that are generalizable, there is also the presumption that the context and setting for delivering the intervention should likewise be generalizable to the intended program users. In Phase V studies, effective Phase IV interventions are translated into large-scale demonstration projects. The major concern is implementation fidelity of an intervention that will now be introduced within even broader populations, including entire communities. This final phase (dissemination research), where collaboration and coordination with various community partners is likely to receive even greater attention, is intended to provide the necessary data and experience to move interventions into public health service programs at the national, regional, state, and local levels.

Greenwald and Cullen specifically advocated that intervention research unfold in a systematic fashion, building on and extending the body of science accumulated in previous phases. By explicitly defining the difference between Phase III and Phase IV research as being an emphasis on internal control versus representativeness, both Flay and Greenwald and Cullen assumed that successful Phase III trials would lead naturally to Phase IV trials. Unfortunately, this has not occurred.1,11,20 Instead, we currently find ourselves in a situation in which we have many small-scale efficacy studies of unknown generalizability and few successful effectiveness trials.21,22 In particular, we know very little about the representativeness of participants, settings, or intervention agents participating in health promotion research.1,21

Although the National Cancer Institute no longer emphasizes this linear "phases of research" model,23,24 the model was extremely influential in guiding an entire generation of research; many researchers, reviewers, and editors still use this framework when designing, funding, and evaluating research—and in deciding what types of studies are needed to advance a given area. Similar phase models are influential in evaluating prevention effectiveness25 and in developing drug therapies. In the remainder of this article, we discuss how this well-intentioned and logical phase of research paradigm may have fallen short of its intended goal, and propose approaches to remedy the present situation.

Our primary thesis is that this "trickle-down" model of how to translate research into practice—namely, that the optimal way to develop disseminable interventions is to progress from efficacy studies to effectiveness trials to dissemination projects—is inherently flawed, or at least incomplete. We posit that given the respective cultures, values, and methodological traditions that have developed within efficacy versus population-based effectiveness research, it is highly unlikely that interventions that are successful in efficacy studies will do well in effectiveness studies, or in real-world applications.

Table 1Go summarizes the key characteristics of well-designed efficacy and effectiveness trials, using the RE-AIM evaluation framework.26,27 This model for evaluating interventions is intended to refocus priorities on public health issues, and it gives balanced emphasis to internal and external validity (see http://www.re-aim.org). RE-AIM is an acronym for Reach, Efficacy or Effectiveness (depending on the stage of research), Adoption, Implementation, and Maintenance.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
TABLE 1— Distinctive Characteristics of Efficacy and Effectiveness Intervention Studies, Using RE-AIM26,27 Dimensions for Program Evaluation

Reach refers to the participation rate among those approached and the representativeness of participants. Factors determining reach are the size and characteristics of the potential audience and the barriers to participation (e.g., cost, social and environmental context, necessary referrals, transportation, and inconvenience). Efficacy or effectiveness pertains to the impact of an intervention on specified outcome criteria and includes measures of potential negative outcomes as well as intended results (as recommended by Flay,18 but seldom collected)28,29 (D. A. Dzewaltowski et al., unpublished data, 2002). Adoption operates at the setting level and concerns the percentage and representativeness of organizations or settings that will conduct a given program. Rogers30 has written extensively on adoption and dissemination issues. Factors associated with adoption include political and cultural fit, cost, level of resources and expertise required, and how similar a proposed service is to current practices of an organization. Implementation refers to intervention integrity, or the quality and consistency of delivery. Finally, maintenance operates at both the individual and the setting or organizational level. At the individual level, maintenance refers to how well behavior changes hold up in the long term. At the setting level, it refers to the extent to which a treatment or practice becomes institutionalized in an organization.

Table 1Go summarizes how the RE-AIM dimensions apply to the efficacy–effectiveness distinction. Efficacy trials typically limit reach by seeking motivated, homogeneous participants with minimal or no complications or comorbidities. The considerable degree of initial screening for eligibility inherently limits the reach of an efficacy trial. Adoption is often treated as a nonissue for efficacy trials so long as at least one or, in some trials, a few settings are willing to participate. For effectiveness trials, reach is usually higher because participants are drawn from a broad and "defined" population. Adoption is critical because the settings need to commit their own resources and expect the intervention to "fit" with existing procedures.

Implementation in an efficacy trial is usually accomplished by research staff following a standardized protocol, whereas in an effectiveness trial, regular staff with many competing demands on their time must implement the intervention. While such staff are also guided by a protocol, adherence is likely to be more variable.1 Because they are implemented by research staff, efficacy interventions are often more complex and intensive than effectiveness interventions. Maintenance is usually a nonissue for efficacy trials at the setting level; it is expected that the intervention will cease when final assessments are completed and research staff depart. Since effectiveness trials are intended to represent typical setting conditions, it is hoped that the intervention will be maintained, assuming there are positive results.


 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

We conclude that the characteristics that cause an intervention to be successful in efficacy research (e.g., intensive, complex, highly standardized) are fundamentally different from, and often at odds with, programs that succeed in population-based effectiveness settings (e.g., having broad appeal, being adaptable for both participants and intervention agents). If this is the case, then the "system" of moving from research to usual service programs, to which we have subscribed, may be broken and may need to be substantially modified.

Why does this linear progression of research, which is analogous to the steps used successfully to evaluate and bring pharmaceuticals to market, seem to fail with behavioral and health promotion research? One contextual factor is that, before trials, pharmaceutical companies invest considerable time and money establishing that the drug affects relevant biological mediators to a much greater extent than behavioral researchers invest in showing that their interventions affect psychosocial mediators. Granted, industry has vastly more resources. But we suggest that key differences also reside in the nature of the interventions.

Standard medical interventions (e.g., drugs or surgery) are presumed to be robust, readily transferable from setting to setting, and to work approximately equally across broad categories of patients. Clinicians exercise discretion about dosage and surgeons vary in experience, but it is still presumed that the pill is the same whoever administers it. Medicinal and surgical protocols can be relatively precisely defined, and adherence to them can be more easily monitored relative to behavioral interventions. Behavioral interventions are more difficult to define and standardize in part because of the inherent interactivity with client characteristics, preferences, and behaviors. This is exacerbated when behavioral interventions are delivered by staff whose training and expertise fall outside of behavioral science. In efficacy trials, research staff usually bring expertise in behavioral intervention and ensure that it is implemented consistently. This level of quality control and standardization is typically absent among regular health care staff implementing interventions for effectiveness trials.

There are 2 underlying differences between efficacy and effectiveness approaches that we feel are responsible for the current state of affairs. The first is that in an effort to enhance internal validity and control extraneous factors, the tradition in efficacy studies has been to simplify and narrow settings, conditions, participants, and a variety of other factors. There is nothing inherently wrong with this methodological approach, and the tradition of reductionism (e.g., understanding effects by isolating them and removing or controlling other factors) has contributed much to the advancement of science and theory.31 The problem is that usually the longer-range intent is to generalize beyond the narrow conditions of the efficacy trial. In effectiveness trials, an intervention must be robust across a variety of different participants, settings, conditions, and other less controlled factors. Equally important, it must appeal to a broad "defined population" or target audience.

A classic example of the typical differences between a health care efficacy study and an effectiveness trial concerns subject selection. In a tightly controlled efficacy trial, only highly motivated, homogenous self-selected volunteers who do not have any complications or other comorbid conditions are eligible (to control for potential confounding factors). Then, following success in such an efficacy study, we expect the same intervention to appeal to and be effective in a much broader cross-section of participants, many of whom have comorbid conditions and may not volunteer for treatment.

The second key difference between efficacy and effectiveness trials concerns how settings and contextual factors are treated. In efficacy studies, the usual approach is to control variance by restricting the setting to one set of circumstances—for example, one particular clinic (which often includes intervention experts). In contrast, a key characteristic of effectiveness trials is to produce robust effects and to understand variation in outcomes across heterogeneous settings and delivery agents. Therefore, it should not be surprising when the results of an intervention are efficacious under a highly specific set of circumstances but fail to replicate across a wide variety of settings, conditions, and intervention agents in effectiveness research.


 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER MEET?
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

From the above discussion, it may seem hopeless to expect congruence across findings from efficacy and effectiveness studies. Some might go so far as to suggest, as one reviewer of this manuscript did, that perhaps efficacy studies should be abandoned altogether. We are optimistic, however, that there are solutions to the present disconnect. In brief, we need to embrace and study the complexity of the world, rather than attempting to ignore or reduce it by studying only isolated (and often unrepresentative) situations.32 What is needed is a "science of larger social units"33 that takes into account and analyzes the social context(s) in which experiments are conducted. To advance our present state of science, the question that we need to ask of both efficacy and effectiveness studies is "What are the characteristics of interventions that can (a) reach large numbers of people, especially those who can most benefit, (b) be broadly adopted by different settings (worksite, school, health, or community), (c) be consistently implemented by different staff members with moderate levels of training and expertise, and (d) produce replicable and long-lasting effects (and minimal negative impacts) at a reasonable cost?"

This suggested focus has important implications. It implies that we need to consider not only individual participants but also the settings within which they reside and receive treatment. This move to a multilevel approach is consistent with developments in several fields, and methodologies for how to handle such factors are available. There is not only a rich conceptual history to the study of generalization34 and of representative or purposeful sampling,35,36 but also statistical methods for handling these contextual factors.37

This comes down to an issue of generalization.38 The prevailing view seems to be that efficacy studies should focus only on internal validity and theoretical process mechanisms, and that issues of external validity should be left until later effectiveness studies. In contrast, we argue that issues of moderating variables (external validity) need to be addressed in both efficacy and effectiveness studies. Brewer39 conceptualizes such social context factors as moderating variables that influence the conclusions that can be drawn about the efficacy of an intervention. Moderating variables (e.g., race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, type of setting or intervention agent) are relatively stable factors that interact with the intervention or change the effect of the program. Researchers should consider elevating hypotheses related to moderator variables to primary aims.


 WHAT CAN BE DONE? DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

It is difficult to change established practice patterns, regardless of whether they be of clinicians, researchers, or funding agencies. It cannot reasonably be expected that many scientists will quickly discontinue practices in which they have been trained and become comfortable. It is also more efficient, and much more under one’s control, to continue to conduct efficacy studies without considering moderating variables or external validity because "the purpose is to study interventions under ideal conditions." However, as illustrated above, , this is only true if one does not intend to generalize one’s conclusions beyond the very limited sample and conditions of a given study,1,31 which is hardly ever the case in health promotion research.

There is an increasingly well-documented disparity between the large amount of information on efficacy and the very small amount of information on effectiveness and representativeness.21,22,40 To produce significant improvement in the current state of affairs, changes will be necessary on the part of researchers, funding organizations, journal reviewers, and grant review panels. We propose 4 specific changes—2 of which focus on researchers, 1 on journal editors, and 1 on funding organizations.

1. Researchers should pay increased attention to moderating factors in both efficacy and effectiveness research. Table 2Go outlines how data collection and information about moderating factors, such as participant characteristics (reach) and setting characteristics (adoption), can be incorporated into both efficacy and effectiveness research in a manner appropriate to that phase. Using the RE-AIM framework, we suggest that researchers consider the types of settings, intervention agents, and individuals that they wish their program to be used by when designing and evaluating interventions. During efficacy studies, purposeful or oversampling strategies can be used to include both specific end-user groups (e.g., minorities, less educated) and settings of interest. A critical concern for broader application—and an integral part of Flay’s original description18—was measurement of potential harmful outcomes. This part of his definition has seldom been addressed, but it needs to be.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
TABLE 2— Ways to Address RE-AIM26,27 Issues in Efficacy and Effectiveness Studies

Participatory research methods, including developing one’s intervention ideas collaboratively with members of the intended audience (individuals, intervention agents, and organization decisionmakers) should not be left for later phases of research but built into efficacy studies. More formal measures of adoption and setting level maintenance may need to wait until later effectiveness studies (Table 2Go), but both qualitative and quantitative "proxy measures" of these factors can and should be addressed in efficacy studies. Such information can lead to better tailoring of interventions to organizational culture in the same way that tailoring of intervention at the individual level has led to increased success.41,42 A final recommendation for both efficacy and effectiveness studies is to include a variety of intervention agents, to describe their backgrounds and levels of experience/expertise with regard to the target behavior, and to report on potential differences in implementation and outcomes associated with these differences.43

As illustrated in Table 2Go, issues pertaining to moderating factors—and eventual translation into practice—are best addressed during the planning phases of research. RE-AIM, or other evaluation models,13,16 can be used to help plan and select samples, interventions, settings, and agents in ways that make it more likely that results will be replicated in later studies.

2. Realize that public health impact involves more than just efficacy. Our training and current review criteria all emphasize producing large effect sizes under tightly controlled conditions. To make a real-world impact, several other criteria are also necessary.

a. At the individual level, several research groups have proposed that Impact = Reach (R) x Efficacy (E).44–47 It is not enough to produce a highly efficacious intervention. To have broad public health impact, an intervention must also have high reach. To the Impact = R x E formula, we would add a third component: implementation (I). As discussed by Basch et al.,19 a program cannot be effective if it is not implemented. Thus, we propose that individual-level Impact = R x E x I.

b. An individual-level focus is, however, not sufficient. An intervention also has to be acceptable to and adopted by a variety of intervention settings, and to be implemented relatively consistently by different intervention agents. In other words, the parallel setting or organizational-level impact formula should be Organizational Impact (OI) = Adoption (A) x Implementation (I). Several authors have discussed issues of nesting and setting factors37,48 and how to adjust individual-level effects for issues of nonindependence. However, to our knowledge, the A x I = OI formula for estimating the impact of an intervention across settings has not been discussed, with the exception of an early related proposal by Kolbe49 that Impact = Effectiveness x Dissemination x Maintenance. It is important to emphasize that in terms of overall public health effect, adoption and implementation are as important as reach and efficacy, and that we need more emphasis on studies of organizational- and system-level factors.

3. Include external validity reporting criteria in author guidelines. Within medicine, a widely agreed upon set of criteria for reporting the results of randomized clinical trials has been developed. Known as the CONSORT criteria,50 these reporting standards have been widely adopted by leading medical journals and have helped to increase the quality of published research. As helpful as the CONSORT criteria are, they are almost exclusively concerned with issues of internal validity. Only 1 out of 22 recommendations directly addresses external validity issues51; in contrast to the other very specific and concrete criteria, it simply states "Generalizability (external validity) of the trial findings" and provides no guidance as to how this issue should be reported.

We propose the following 7 additions to the existing CONSORT criteria, which would help greatly to increase awareness of and reporting on external validity. If such criteria were widely adopted, it would greatly enhance the quality and information value not only of individual studies but also of evidence-based reviews and meta-analyses. The current state of health promotion research is so biased toward reporting on internal validity issues that it is difficult to draw any conclusions about generalization. In particular, there has been a serious lack of attention to issues of representativeness, especially at the level of settings and intervention agents.21,28,52 This becomes even more problematic when the evidence upon which meta-analyses and practice recommendations are based consists largely or solely of efficacy studies of unknown generalizability.

The 7 items that we propose below should apply to both efficacy and effectiveness studies. They would not require a great deal of additional journal space and are described below in the same format as existing CONSORT items. These criteria were recently added by the Evidence-Based Behavioral Medicine Committee of the Society of Behavioral Medicine53 to their recommendations for reporting on behavioral intervention studies.

a. State the target population to which the study intends to generalize.

b. Report the rate of exclusions, the participation rate among those eligible, and the representativeness of participants.

c. Report on methods of recruiting study settings, including exclusion rate, participation rate among those approached, and representativeness of settings studied.

d. Describe the participation rate and characteristics of those delivering the intervention. State the population of intervention agents that one would see eventually implementing the program and how the study interventionists compare with those who will eventually deliver the intervention.

e. Report the extent to which different components of the intervention are delivered (by different intervention agents) as intended in the protocol.

f. Report the specific time, and costs required to deliver the intervention.

g. Report on organizational level of continuance, discontinuance or adaptation in modified form of the intervention once the trial is completed, and individual-level maintenance of results.

We think that such information should be of relevance not only to researchers but also to clinicians, health directors, and decisionmakers responsible for selecting prevention and health promotion programs. In fact, we think that these parties already make implicit use of these dimensions. Making them explicit should aid reading of the literature and guide more informed program selections.

4. Increase funding for research focused on moderating variables, external validity, and robustness. The large imbalance between the extent to which health promotion investigations focus on internal validity and the extent to which they focus on external validity will not be remedied without substantial changes in funding priorities. Table 3Go lists several recommendations for funding organizations that would help correct this imbalance.


View this table:
[in this window]
[in a new window]
TABLE 3— Recommendations for Funding Organizations to Accelerate Transfer of Research to Practice

These recommendations would have 2 effects. The first would be to increase the small number of well-conducted effectiveness studies now available. The second would be to increase the relevance of efficacy studies for practice by focusing attention on moderating variables and the range of conditions, settings, intervention agents, and participants to which the results apply. Such refocused funding priorities should also increase understanding of health disparities and help reduce them, since more research would be conducted involving minorities and low-income settings. Finally, funding organizations might explicitly have reviewers rate proposals on their likely robustness or potential for widespread application and impact. This could be done by methods described in the Guide to Community Preventive Services.54


 CONCLUSIONS
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

In summary, at least part of the reason for the slow and uneven translation of research findings into practice in the health promotion sciences is lack of attention to issues of generalization and external validity (moderating factors that potentially limit the robustness of interventions). There also needs to be a greater understanding of, and research on, setting-level social contextual factors.16,55,56 If these issues were addressed in the design and reporting of efficacy as well as effectiveness studies, it would greatly advance the current quality of research and our knowledge base. These issues are to a large extent under the control of researchers, reviewers, and funding organizations, and we have listed actions that each of these parties can take to facilitate better transfer from efficacy to effectiveness research.


 Acknowledgments

This project was supported by The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (grant 030102) and the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (grant HS10123).

We acknowledge the contributions of Allan Best, PhD, Brian Flay, PhD, Lisa Klesges, PhD, and Thomas M. Vogt, MD, MPH, for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.


 Footnotes

Contributors

All authors produced original drafts of sections of the manuscript, extensively edited each other’s contributions, and made substantive contributions to the ideas expressed in the manuscript.

Peer Reviewed

Accepted for publication October 24, 2002.


 References
 TOP
 ABSTRACT
 INTRODUCTION
 EFFICACY AND EFFECTIVENESS...
 WHY THE DISCONNECT?
 SHALL THE TWAIN EVER...
 WHAT CAN BE DONE?...
 CONCLUSIONS
 References

1. Clark GN. Improving the transition from basic efficacy research to effectiveness studies: methodological issues and procedures. J Consult Clin Psychol.1995;63:718–725.[ISI][Medline]

2. Weisz JR, Weisz B, Donenberg GR. The lab versus the clinic: effects of child and adolescent psychotherapy. Am Psychol.1992;47:1578–1585.[Medline]

3. Briss PA, Zaza S, Papaioanou M, et al. Developing an evidence-based Guide to Community Preventive Services—methods. Prev Med.2000;18(suppl 1):35–43.[ISI][Medline]

4. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. The Guide to Community Preventive Services. 2002. Available at: http://www.thecommunityguide.org. Accessed March 11, 2003.

5. Whitlock EP, Orleans CT, Prender N, Allan J. Evaluating primary care behavioral counseling interventions: an evidence-based approach. Am J Prev Med.2002;22:267–284.[ISI][Medline]

6. Department of Health and Human Services. Healthy People 2000. 2002. Available at: http://www.health.gov/healthypeople/data/PROGRVW/default.htm. Accessed March 11, 2003.

7. Smedley BD, Syme SL. Promoting health: intervention strategies from social and behavioral research. Am J Health Promot.2001;15:149–166.[ISI][Medline]

8. Integration of Health Behavior Counseling Into Routine Medical Care. Washington, DC: Center for the Advancement of Health; 2001.

9. Committee on Quality Health Care in America. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2001.

10. Joyner L, McNeeley S, Kahn R. ADA’s provider recognition program. HMO Pract.1997;11:168–170.[Medline]

11. Glasgow RE, Strycker LA. Level of preventive practices for diabetes management: patient, physician, and office correlates in two primary care samples. Am J Prev Med.2000;19:9–14.[ISI][Medline]

12. Health Behavior Change in Managed Care: A Status Report. Washington, DC: Center for the Advancement of Health; 2000.

13. Kottke TE, Edwards BS, Hagen PT. Counseling: implementing our knowledge in a hurried and complex world. Am J Prev Med.1999;17:295–298.[Medline]

14. Woolf SH, Atkins D. The evolving role of prevention in health care contributions of the US Preventive Services Task Force. Am J Prev Med.2001;20:13–20.[Medline]

15. Orlandi MA. Promoting health and preventing disease in health care settings: an analysis of barriers. Prev Med.1987;16:119–130.[ISI][Medline]

16. Green LW. From research to "best practices" in other settings and populations. Am J Health Behav.2001;25:165–178.[ISI][Medline]

17. Greenwald P, Cullen JW. The new emphasis in cancer control. J Natl Cancer Inst.1985;74:543–551.

18. Flay BR. Efficacy and effectiveness trials (and other phases of research) in the development of health promotion programs. Prev Med.1986;15:451–474.[ISI][Medline]

19. Basch CE, Sliepcevich EM, Gold RS. Avoiding type III errors in health education program evaluations. Health Educ Q.1985;12:315–331.[ISI][Medline]

20. King AC. The coming of age of behavioral research in physical activity. Ann Behav Med. 2001;23:227–228.[Medline]

21. Glasgow RE, Bull SS, Gillette C, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA. Behavior change intervention research in health care settings: a review of recent reports with emphasis on external validity. Am J Prev Med.2002;23:62–69.[ISI][Medline]

22. Oldenburg B, Ffrench BF, Sallis JF. Health behavior research: the quality of the evidence base. Am J Health Promot.2000;14:253–257.[ISI][Medline]

23. Hiatt RA, Rimer BK. A new strategy for cancer control research. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev.1999;8:957–964.[Free Full Text]

24. Kerner JF. Closing the Gap Between Discovery and Delivery. Washington, DC: National Cancer Institute; 2002.

25. Teutsch SM. A framework for assessing the effectiveness of disease and injury prevention. MMWR Recomm Rep.1992;41(RR-3):1–12.[Medline]

26. Glasgow RE, Vogt TM, Boles SM. Evaluating the public health impact of health promotion interventions: the RE-AIM framework. Am J Public Health.1999;89:1322–1327.[Abstract/Free Full Text]

27. Glasgow RE, McKay HG, Piette JD, Reynolds KD. The RE-AIM framework for evaluating interventions: what can it tell us about approaches to chronic illness management? Patient Educ Couns.2001;44:119–127.[ISI][Medline]

28. Glasgow RE, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA, Bull SS, Estabrooks P. The future of health behavior change research: what is needed to improve translation of research into health promotion practice? Ann Behav Med. In press.

29. Estabrooks PA, Dzewaltowski DA, Glasgow RE, Klesges LM. How well has recent literature reported on important issues related to translating school-based health promotion research into practice? J School Health.2003;73:21–28.[Medline]

30. Rogers EM. Diffusion of Innovations. 4th ed. New York, NY: Free Press; 1995.

31. Mook DG. In defense of external invalidity. Am Psychol.1983;38:379–387.

32. Axelrod R, Cohen MD. Harnessing Complexity: Organizational Implications of a Scientific Frontier. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster; 2000.

33. Biglan A, Glasgow RE, Singer G. The need for a science of larger social units: a contextual approach. Behav Ther.1990;21:195–215.

34. Gleser GC, Cronbach LJ, Rajaratnam N. Generalizability of scores influenced by multiple sources of variance. Psychometrika.1965;30:1373–1385.

35. Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell PT. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Design for Generalized Causal Inference. Boston, Mass: Houghton Mifflin; 2002.

36. Brunswik E. Representative design and probabilistic theory in functional psychology. Psychol Rev.1955;62:217.

37. Murray DM. Statistical models appropriate for designs often used in group-randomized trials. Stat Med.2001;20:1373–1385.[ISI][Medline]

38. Cook TD, Campbell DT. Quasi-Experimentation: Design and Analysis Issues for Field Settings. Chicago, Ill: Rand McNally; 1979.

39. Brewer MB. Research design and issues of validity. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, eds. Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2000:3–39.

40. Oldenburg BF, Sallis JF, Ffrench ML, Owen N. Health promotion research and the diffusion and institutionalization of interventions. Health Educ Res.1999;14:121–130.[Abstract/Free Full Text]

41. Skinner CS, Campbell MK, Rimer BK, Curry S, Prochaska JO. How effective is tailored print communication? Ann Behav Med.1999;21:290–298.[ISI][Medline]

42. Kreuter MW, Strecher VJ, Glassman B. One size does not fit all: the case for tailoring print materials. Ann Behav Med.1999;21:276–283.[ISI][Medline]

43. Glasgow RE, Toobert DJ, Hampson SE, Strycker LA. Implementation, generalization, and long-term results of the "Choosing Well" diabetes self-management intervention. Patient Educ Couns.2002;48:115–122.[ISI][Medline]

44. Abrams DB, Emmons KM, Linnan L, Biener L. Smoking cessation at the workplace: conceptual and practical considerations. In: Richmond R, ed. Interventions for Smokers: An International Perspective. New York, NY: Williams & Wilkins; 1994:137–169.

45. Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Fava JL, Rossi JS, Tsoh JY. Evaluating a population-based recruitment approach and a stage-based expert system intervention for smoking cessation. Addict Behav.2001;26:583–602.[ISI][Medline]

46. Jeffery RW. Risk behaviors and health: contrasting individual and population perspectives. Am Psychol.1989;44:1194–1202.[Medline]

47. Lichtenstein E, Glasgow RE. A pragmatic framework for smoking cessation: implications for clinical and public health programs. Psychol Addict Behav.1997;11:142–151.

48. Elbourne DR, Campbell MK. Extending the CONSORT statement to cluster randomized trials: for discussion. Stat Med.2001;20:489–496.[ISI][Medline]

49. Kolbe LJ. Increasing the impact of school health promotion programs: emerging research perspectives. Health Educ.1986;17:49–52.[Medline]

50. Moher D, Schulz KF, Altman D. The CONSORT statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports. JAMA.2001;285:1987–1991.[Abstract/Free Full Text]

51. Zaza S, Lawrence RS, Mahan CS, Fullilove M, et al. Scope and organization of the Guide to Community Preventive Services. Task Force on Community Preventive Services. Am J Prev Med.2000;18(suppl 1):27–34.

52. Bull SS, Gillette C, Glasgow RE, Estabrooks P. Worksite health promotion research: to what extent can we generalize the results and what is needed to translate research to practice? Health Educ Behav. In press.

53. Davidson K, Goldstein M, Kaplan R, et al. Evidence-based behavioral medicine: what is it and how do we get there? Ann Behav Med. In press.

54. Green LW, Kreuter MW. Commentary on the emerging Guide to Community Preventive Services from a health promotion perspective. Am J Prev Med.2000;18:7–9.[ISI][Medline]

55. Institute of Medicine. Promoting Health: Intervention Strategies From Social and Behavioral Research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000.

56. Green LM, Kreuter MW. Health Promotion Planning: An Educational and Ecological Approach. 3rd ed. Mountain View, Calif: Mayfield Publishing Co; 1999.




This article has been cited by other articles:


Home page
Am J Crit CareHome page
K. Horn, G. Dino, C. Hamilton, N. Noerachmanto, and J. Zhang
Feasibility of a smoking cessation intervention for teens in the emergency department: reach, implementation fidelity, and acceptability.
Am. J. Crit. Care., May 1, 2008; 17(3): 205 - 216.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
JNCI J Natl Cancer InstHome page
D. J. del Junco, S. W. Vernon, S. P. Coan, J. A. Tiro, L. A. Bastian, L. S. Savas, C. A. Perz, D. R. Lairson, W. Chan, C. Warrick, et al.
Promoting Regular Mammography Screening I. A Systematic Assessment of Validity in a Randomized Trial
J Natl Cancer Inst, March 5, 2008; 100(5): 333 - 346.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Journal of Applied GerontologyHome page
J. C. Frank, C. P. Coviak, T. C. Healy, B. Belza, and B. L. Casado
Addressing Fidelity in Evidence-Based Health Promotion Programs for Older Adults
Journal of Applied Gerontology, February 1, 2008; 27(1): 4 - 33.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
A. Steckler and K. R. McLeroy
The Importance of External Validity
Am J Public Health, January 1, 2008; 98(1): 9 - 10.
[Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Cancer Epidemiol. Biomarkers Prev.Home page
R. A. Hiatt, S. M. Miller, and S. W. Vernon
Translational Research and Good Behavior
Cancer Epidemiol. Biomarkers Prev., November 1, 2007; 16(11): 2184 - 2185.
[Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
The Diabetes EducatorHome page
E. B. Fisher, C. T. Thorpe, B. M. DeVellis, and R. F. DeVellis
Healthy Coping, Negative Emotions, and Diabetes Management: A Systematic Review and Appraisal
The Diabetes Educator, November 1, 2007; 33(6): 1080 - 1103.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
C. Escoffery, K. Glanz, and T. Elliott
Process evaluation of the Pool Cool Diffusion Trial for skin cancer prevention across 2 years
Health Educ. Res., October 22, 2007; (2007) cym060v1.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Diabetes CareHome page
P. Absetz, R. Valve, B. Oldenburg, H. Heinonen, A. Nissinen, M. Fogelholm, V. Ilvesmaki, M. Talja, and A. Uutela
Type 2 Diabetes Prevention in the "Real World": One-year results of the GOAL Implementation Trial
Diabetes Care, October 1, 2007; 30(10): 2465 - 2470.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
C. H. Ravesloot, T Seekins, T Cahill, S Lindgren, D. E. Nary, and G White
Health promotion for people with disabilities: development and evaluation of the Living Well with a Disability program
Health Educ. Res., August 1, 2007; 22(4): 522 - 531.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
ANN INTERN MEDHome page
M. L. Dansinger, A. Tatsioni, J. B. Wong, M. Chung, and E. M. Balk
Meta-analysis: The Effect of Dietary Counseling for Weight Loss
Ann Intern Med, July 3, 2007; 147(1): 41 - 50.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Ann Fam MedHome page
A. J. Dietrich, J. N. Tobin, A. Cassells, C. M. Robinson, M. Reh, K. A. Romero, A. B. Flood, and M. L. Beach
Translation of an Efficacious Cancer-Screening Intervention to Women Enrolled in a Medicaid Managed Care Organization
Ann. Fam. Med, July 1, 2007; 5(4): 320 - 327.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Clin TrialsHome page
M. McFall, A. J. Saxon, S. Thaneemit-Chen, M. W. Smith, A. M. Joseph, T. P. Carmody, J. C. Beckham, C. A. Malte, J. E. Vertrees, K. D. Boardman, et al.
Integrating smoking cessation into mental health care for post-traumatic stress disorder
Clinical Trials, April 1, 2007; 4(2): 178 - 189.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
R. Cameron, S. Manske, K. S. Brown, M. A. Jolin, D. Murnaghan, and C. Lovato
Integrating Public Health Policy, Practice, Evaluation, Surveillance, and Research: The School Health Action Planning and Evaluation System
Am J Public Health, April 1, 2007; 97(4): 648 - 654.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. PsychiatryHome page
A. P. Weiss
Measuring the Impact of Medical Research: Moving From Outputs to Outcomes
Am J Psychiatry, February 1, 2007; 164(2): 206 - 214.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
D. Segaar, C Bolman, M. Willemsen, and H De Vries
Identifying determinants of protocol adoption by midwives: a comprehensive approach
Health Educ. Res., February 1, 2007; 22(1): 14 - 26.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Sex. Transm. Infect.Home page
C A Rietmeijer
Risk reduction counselling for prevention of sexually transmitted infections: how it works and how to make it work
Sex. Transm. Inf., February 1, 2007; 83(1): 2 - 9.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ BehavHome page
M. K. Hunt, E. M. Barbeau, R. Lederman, A. M. Stoddard, C. Chetkovich, R. Goldman, L. Wallace, and G. Sorensen
Process Evaluation Results From the Healthy Directions-Small Business Study
Health Educ Behav, February 1, 2007; 34(1): 90 - 107.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
J Health PsycholHome page
J. O. Prochaska, K. E. Evers, J. M. Prochaska, D. Van Marter, and J. L. Johnson
Efficacy and Effectiveness Trials: Examples from Smoking Cessation and Bullying Prevention
J Health Psychol, January 1, 2007; 12(1): 170 - 178.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Research on Social Work PracticeHome page
J. S. Brekke, K. Ell, and L. A. Palinkas
Translational Science at the National Institute of Mental Health: Can Social Work Take Its Rightful Place?
Research on Social Work Practice, January 1, 2007; 17(1): 123 - 133.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
C. M. Lyles, L. S. Kay, N. Crepaz, J. H. Herbst, W. F. Passin, A. S. Kim, S. M. Rama, S. Thadiparthi, J. B. DeLuca, M. M. Mullins, et al.
Best-Evidence Interventions: Findings From a Systematic Review of HIV Behavioral Interventions for US Populations at High Risk, 2000-2004
Am J Public Health, January 1, 2007; 97(1): 133 - 143.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
JAMAHome page
D. E. Jorenby, S. Tonstad, and D. Gonzales
Efficacy of Varenicline for Smoking Cessation--Reply
JAMA, December 6, 2006; 296(21): 2555 - 2556.
[Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Inj. Prev.Home page
S Mallonee, C Fowler, and G R Istre
Bridging the gap between research and practice: a continuing challenge
Inj. Prev., December 1, 2006; 12(6): 357 - 359.
[Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Psychiatr. Serv.Home page
J. A. Cook
Employment Barriers for Persons With Psychiatric Disabilities: Update of a Report for the President's Commission
Psychiatr Serv, October 1, 2006; 57(10): 1391 - 1405.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
R. E. Glasgow, L. M. Klesges, D. A. Dzewaltowski, P. A. Estabrooks, and T. M. Vogt
Evaluating the impact of health promotion programs: using the RE-AIM framework to form summary measures for decision making involving complex issues
Health Educ. Res., October 1, 2006; 21(5): 688 - 694.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
GerontologistHome page
G. E. Alkema and D. E. Alley
Gerontology's Future: An Integrative Model for Disciplinary Advancement.
Gerontologist, October 1, 2006; 46(5): 574 - 582.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
American Journal of EvaluationHome page
L. H. Jaycox, D. F. McCaffrey, B. W. Ocampo, G. A. Shelley, S. M. Blake, D. J. Peterson, L. S. Richmond, and J. E. Kub
Challenges in the Evaluation and Implementation of School-Based Prevention and Intervention Programs on Sensitive Topics
American Journal of Evaluation, September 1, 2006; 27(3): 320 - 336.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Eval Health ProfHome page
L. A. Rohrbach, R. Grana, S. Sussman, and T. W. Valente
Type II Translation: Transporting Prevention Interventions From Research to Real-World Settings.
Eval Health Prof, September 1, 2006; 29(3): 302 - 333.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Eval Health ProfHome page
E. M. Ginexi and T. F. Hilton
What's Next for Translation Research?
Eval Health Prof, September 1, 2006; 29(3): 334 - 347.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
ptjournalHome page
G. P Brennan, J. M Fritz, and S. J Hunter
Impact of Continuing Education Interventions on Clinical Outcomes of Patients With Neck Pain Who Received Physical Therapy
Physical Therapy, September 1, 2006; 86(9): 1251 - 1262.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ BehavHome page
D. B. Abrams
Applying transdisciplinary research strategies to understanding and eliminating health disparities.
Health Educ Behav, August 1, 2006; 33(4): 515 - 531.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Psychiatr. Serv.Home page
M. S. Bauer, L. McBride, W. O. Williford, H. Glick, B. Kinosian, L. Altshuler, T. Beresford, A. M. Kilbourne, M. Sajatovic, and and Coauthors for the Cooperative Studies Program
Collaborative Care for Bipolar Disorder: Part I. Intervention and Implementation in a Randomized Effectiveness Trial
Psychiatr Serv, July 1, 2006; 57(7): 927 - 936.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Psychiatr. Serv.Home page
M. S. Bauer, L. McBride, W. O. Williford, H. Glick, B. Kinosian, L. Altshuler, T. Beresford, A. M. Kilbourne, M. Sajatovic, and and Coauthors for the Cooperative Studies Program
Collaborative Care for Bipolar Disorder: Part II. Impact on Clinical Outcome, Function, and Costs
Psychiatr Serv, July 1, 2006; 57(7): 937 - 945.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
S. Audrey, J. Holliday, N. Parry-Langdon, and R. Campbell
Meeting the challenges of implementing process evaluation within randomized controlled trials: the example of ASSIST (A Stop Smoking in Schools Trial)
Health Educ. Res., July 1, 2006; 21(3): 366 - 377.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
S. Wilcox, M. Dowda, S. F. Griffin, C. Rheaume, M. G. Ory, L. Leviton, A. C. King, A. Dunn, D. M. Buchner, T. Bazzarre, et al.
Results of the First Year of Active for Life: Translation of 2 Evidence-Based Physical Activity Programs for Older Adults Into Community Settings
Am J Public Health, July 1, 2006; 96(7): 1201 - 1209.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Eval Health ProfHome page
K. D. Reynolds and D. Spruijt-Metz
Translational research in childhood obesity prevention.
Eval Health Prof, June 1, 2006; 29(2): 219 - 245.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Eval Health ProfHome page
S. Sussman, T. W. Valente, L. A. Rohrbach, S. Skara, and M. Ann Pentz
Translation in the health professions: converting science into action.
Eval Health Prof, March 1, 2006; 29(1): 7 - 32.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Eval Health ProfHome page
C. Werch, J. L. Grenard, J. Burnett, J. A. Watkins, S. Ames, and E. Jobli
Translation as a function of modality: the potential of brief interventions.
Eval Health Prof, March 1, 2006; 29(1): 89 - 125.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Eval Health ProfHome page
L. W. Green and R. E. Glasgow
Evaluating the relevance, generalization, and applicability of research: issues in external validation and translation methodology.
Eval Health Prof, March 1, 2006; 29(1): 126 - 153.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
D. Hallfors, P. H. Brodish, S. Khatapoush, V. Sanchez, H. Cho, and A. Steckler
Feasibility of Screening Adolescents for Suicide Risk in "Real-World" High School Settings
Am J Public Health, February 1, 2006; 96(2): 282 - 287.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
J Am Board Fam MedHome page
R. E. Glasgow
RE-AIMing Research for Application: Ways to Improve Evidence for Family Medicine
J Am Board Fam Med, January 1, 2006; 19(1): 11 - 19.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
J Am Board Fam MedHome page
M. A. Handley, H. Hammer, and D. Schillinger
Navigating the Terrain between Research and Practice: A Collaborative Research Network (CRN) Case Study in Diabetes Research
J Am Board Fam Med, January 1, 2006; 19(1): 85 - 92.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
E. E. Tolley and L. J. Severy
Integrating Behavioral and Social Science Research Into Microbicide Clinical Trials: Challenges and Opportunities
Am J Public Health, January 1, 2006; 96(1): 79 - 83.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Policy Politics Nursing PracticeHome page
G. E. Russell and J. Fawcett
The Conceptual Model for Nursing and Health Policy Revisited
Policy Politics Nursing Practice, November 1, 2005; 6(4): 319 - 326.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
The Diabetes EducatorHome page
T. L. Gary, F. Hill-Briggs, M. Batts-Turner, and F. L. Brancati
Translational Research Principles of an Effectiveness Trial for Diabetes Care in an Urban African American Population
The Diabetes Educator, November 1, 2005; 31(6): 880 - 889.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
Y. Robitaille, S. Laforest, M. Fournier, L. Gauvin, M. Parisien, H. Corriveau, F. Trickey, and N. Damestoy
Moving Forward in Fall Prevention: An Intervention to Improve Balance Among Older Adults in Real-World Settings
Am J Public Health, November 1, 2005; 95(11): 2049 - 2056.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
K. Robinson, S. J. Elliott, S. M. Driedger, J. Eyles, J. O'Loughlin, B. Riley, R. Cameron, D. Harvey, and on behalf of the CHHDP Strategic and Research Advi
Using linking systems to build capacity and enhance dissemination in heart health promotion: a Canadian multiple-case study
Health Educ. Res., October 1, 2005; 20(5): 499 - 513.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Schizophr BullHome page
E. Bromley
A Collaborative Approach to Targeted Treatment Development for Schizophrenia: A Qualitative Evaluation of the NIMH-MATRICS Project
Schizophr Bull, October 1, 2005; 31(4): 954 - 961.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Ann Fam MedHome page
B. F. Crabtree, W. L. Miller, A. F. Tallia, D. J. Cohen, B. DiCicco-Bloom, H. E. McIlvain, V. A. Aita, J. G. Scott, P. B. Gregory, K. C. Stange, et al.
Delivery of Clinical Preventive Services in Family Medicine Offices
Ann. Fam. Med, September 1, 2005; 3(5): 430 - 435.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Child MaltreatHome page
D. J. Whitaker, J. R. Lutzker, and G. A. Shelley
Child Maltreatment Prevention Priorities at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
Child Maltreat, August 1, 2005; 10(3): 245 - 259.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Health Promot PractHome page
A. A. Gandelman, S. A. Vogan, and M. M. Dolcini
Assessing HIV Prevention Provider Knowledge of Behavior Science Theory: Building on Existing Intuitive Experience
Health Promot Pract, July 1, 2005; 6(3): 299 - 307.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. PsychiatryHome page
M. McFall, A. J. Saxon, C. E. Thompson, D. Yoshimoto, C. Malte, K. Straits-Troster, E. Kanter, X.-H. A. Zhou, C. M. Dougherty, and B. Steele
Improving the Rates of Quitting Smoking for Veterans With Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
Am J Psychiatry, July 1, 2005; 162(7): 1311 - 1319.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Ann Fam MedHome page
T. Bodenheimer, D. M. Young, K. MacGregor, and J. S. Holtrop
Practice-Based Research in Primary Care: Facilitator of, or Barrier to, Practice Improvement?
Ann. Fam. Med, July 1, 2005; 3(suppl_2): S28 - S32.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Ann Fam MedHome page
A. M. Adelman and M. Graybill
Integrating a Health Coach into Primary Care: Reflections From the Penn State Ambulatory Research Network
Ann. Fam. Med, July 1, 2005; 3(suppl_2): S33 - S35.
[Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Ann Fam MedHome page
J. W. Mold and K. A. Peterson
Primary Care Practice-Based Research Networks: Working at the Interface Between Research and Quality Improvement
Ann. Fam. Med, May 1, 2005; 3(suppl_1): S12 - S20.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ ResHome page
M. C. White, J. P. Tulsky, E. Menendez, S. Arai, J. Goldenson, and L. M. Kawamura
Improving tuberculosis therapy completion after jail: translation of research to practice
Health Educ. Res., April 1, 2005; 20(2): 163 - 174.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Arch Pediatr Adolesc MedHome page
K. J. Coleman, C. L. Tiller, J. Sanchez, E. M. Heath, O. Sy, G. Milliken, and D. A. Dzewaltowski
Prevention of the Epidemic Increase in Child Risk of Overweight in Low-Income Schools: The El Paso Coordinated Approach to Child Health
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med, March 1, 2005; 159(3): 217 - 224.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Health Educ BehavHome page
M. K. Hunt, R. Lederman, A. M. Stoddard, A. D. LaMontagne, D. McLellan, C. Combe, E. Barbeau, and G. Sorensen
Process Evaluation of an Integrated Health Promotion/Occupational Health Model in WellWorks-2
Health Educ Behav, February 1, 2005; 32(1): 10 - 26.
[Abstract] [PDF]


Home page
Diabetes CareHome page
R. E. Glasgow, P. A. Nutting, D. K. King, C. C. Nelson, G. Cutter, B. Gaglio, A. K. Rahm, and H. Whitesides
Randomized Effectiveness Trial of a Computer-Assisted Intervention to Improve Diabetes Care
Diabetes Care, January 1, 2005; 28(1): 33 - 39.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Am. J. Public HealthHome page
B. C. Lee, J. D. Westaby, and R. L. Berg
Impact of a National Rural Youth Health and Safety Initiative: Results from a Randomized Controlled Trial
Am J Public Health, October 1, 2004; 94(10): 1743 - 1749.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
J Pediatr PsycholHome page
J. L. Wiecha, A. M. El Ayadi, B. F. Fuemmeler, J. E. Carter, S. Handler, S. Johnson, N. Strunk, D. Korzec-Ramirez, and S. L. Gortmaker
Diffusion of an Integrated Health Education Program in an Urban School System: Planet Health
J. Pediatr. Psychol., September 1, 2004; 29(6): 467 - 474.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Diabetes CareHome page
A. M. Wolf, M. R. Conaway, J. Q. Crowther, K. Y. Hazen, J. L.Nadler, B. Oneida, and V. E. Bovbjerg
Translating Lifestyle Intervention to Practice in Obese Patients With Type 2 Diabetes: Improving Control with Activity and Nutrition (ICAN) study
Diabetes Care, July 1, 2004; 27(7): 1570 - 1576.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
ANN INTERN MEDHome page
D. Murphy, T. Chapel, and C. Clark
Moving Diabetes Care from Science to Practice: The Evolution of the National Diabetes Prevention and Control Program
Ann Intern Med, June 1, 2004; 140(11): 978 - 984.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Psychiatr. Serv.Home page
R. K. Ries, D. G. Dyck, R. Short, D. Srebnik, A. Fisher, and K. A. Comtois
Outcomes of Managing Disability Benefits Among Patients With Substance Dependence and Severe Mental Illness
Psychiatr Serv, April 1, 2004; 55(4): 445 - 447.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Inj. Prev.Home page
I B Pless
Editorial
Inj. Prev., February 1, 2004; 10(1): 1 - 2.
[Full Text] [PDF]


Home page
Inj. Prev.Home page
L Forsen, A J Sogaard, S Sandvig, A Schuller, U Roed, and C Arstad
Risk of hip fracture in protected and unprotected falls in nursing homes in Norway
Inj. Prev., February 1, 2004; 10(1): 16 - 20.
[Abstract] [Full Text] [PDF]

eLetters:

Read all eLetters

Efficacy to effectiveness: The Role of Private Enterprise in Translating Research to Practice
Mary Jane Rotheram-Borus
AJPH Online, 11 Nov 2003 [Full text]

This Article
Right arrow Abstract Freely available
Right arrow Full Text (PDF)
Right arrow Submit a response
Right arrow View responses
Right arrow Purchase Article
Right arrow View Shopping Cart
Right arrow Alert me when this article is cited
Right arrow Alert me when eLetters are posted
Right arrow Alert me if a correction is posted
Services
Right arrow Similar articles in this journal
Right arrow Similar articles in ISI Web of Science
Right arrow Similar articles in PubMed
Right arrow Alert me to new issues of the journal
Right arrow Download to citation manager
Right arrow reprints & permissions
Citing Articles
Right arrow Citing Articles via HighWire
Right arrow Citing Articles via ISI Web of Science (153)
Right arrow Citing Articles via Google Scholar
Google Scholar
Right arrow Articles by Glasgow, R. E.
Right arrow Articles by Marcus, A. C.
Right arrow Search for Related Content
PubMed
Right arrow PubMed Citation
Right arrow Articles by Glasgow, R. E.
Right arrow Articles by Marcus, A. C.
Related Collections
Right arrow Health Promotion
Right arrow Prevention
Right arrow Health Education


HOME HELP FEEDBACK SUBSCRIPTIONS ARCHIVE SEARCH TABLE OF CONTENTS
Copyright © 2003 by the American Public Health Association

No comments: